Lecture 1
1.1 Gladwin, T. E., Figner, B., Crone, E. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2011).
Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 364-376.
In this review, we will focus on symmetries between adolescence- and addiction related changes in
motivation and cognitive control that may help explain increased the likelihood of substance abuse
and the onset of addiction in adolescence. During adolescence, children increasingly master the
ability to control their behavior for the benefit of longer-term goals.
Risky decision making in adolescence
Adolescents are known to have a tendency to take more and greater risks than individuals in other
age groups in many life domains. This pattern (U-shape) has only recently been demonstrated in
experimental studies, which dissociated between “hot” affect-charged versus “cold” predominantly
deliberative processes in risky choice and risk taking. Studies have shown that heightened
involvement of affective processes in risky decision-making (in the hot CCT) leads to increased risk
taking in adolescents (associated with impoverished use of risk relevant information), compared to
children and adults. In contrast, when risky decisions are made involving mainly deliberative
processes and no or little affect (cold CCT), adolescents show the same levels of risk taking as children
and adults.
Neural developments underlying risky decision making
Insights from anatomical and functional changes in the rodent and the human brain have led to
neurobiological models characterizing the “adolescent brain” by two interacting systems:
1. The first is the relatively early maturing and “hot” affective-motivational bottom-up system
a. Involves subcortical brain areas (dopamine rich areas)
2. The second is the more slowly developing “cold” top-down control system.
a. Involve prefrontal regions, which have been implicated in self-control, planning,
abstract thinking.
The above frontostriatal model of adolescent decision making thus describes a potential for
imbalance in motivational bottom-up versus controlling top-down processes.
Addiction
Current neurobiological research has identified two broad types of neuro-adaptations that result
from repeated alcohol and drug use. First, neural sensitization leads to strong impulsive reactions
(e.g., attentional biases and approach tendencies) to classically conditioned cues that signal alcohol
or drugs. In later phases of addiction, habitual responses become important, with cues automatically
triggering approach-reactions, outside voluntary control. Increased incentive salience may result in
attentional capture (“attentional bias”) of substance-related cues. Finally, negative affect may trigger
an urge to take drugs through associative processes. Second, there is evidence that heavy alcohol
and drug use results in impaired control functions.
Interventions
1. Working memory training
a. Aimed at training the likelihood of successful control
, b. The top-down biasing aspect of working memory, or executive control functions, as
well as control of access to WM are trainable.
c. In addition to behavioral WM training, there is also emerging evidence that low
voltage electrical stimulation at the scalp (transcranial Direct Current Stimulation, or
tDCS) can facilitate dorsolateral prefrontal activity, leading to enhanced working
memory (Fregni et al., 2005) and reductions in craving.
2. Cognitive bias modification
a. Attentional bias modification: For example, in a visual probe test, two stimuli
(pictures or words; usually one related to the problem and one neutral) are
presented at the same time, after which a probe appears on one of the two stimuli
locations. In an assessment instrument, the probe to which people react, appears
equally often in the location of the disorder-related stimulus and the neutral
stimulus.
b. Approach bias modification: The AAT (alcohol Approach Avoidance Task) contains a
“zoom-feature” which has the effect that upon a pull movement, the picture size on
the computer screen increases, and upon a push movement, it decreases, which
generates a strong sense of approach and avoidance, respectively, and
disambiguates the meaning of pushing (is this encoded as “moving towards” or
“pushing away?”) and pulling the joystick. Alcoholics and heavy drinkers have an
approach bias for alcohol.
c. Further developments: For example, evaluative conditioning, neurofeedback
training, and inhibition training.
Conclusion
It was noted that concerns have been raised about the adequacy of dual-process models as a
theoretical foundation. However, such concerns appear to be addressable in principle by carefully
considering levels of description: motivational processes and top-down biasing can be understood as
intertwined, co-developing components of more versus less reflective states of processing. In
practical terms, dual process models of addiction not only provide hypotheses why adolescents are
at increased risk of substance abuse, but have also suggested interventions that have already been
successfully applied in adults. In conclusion, the integration of motivation and control appears critical
not only to the development of individuals, but also to the further development of theory in
cognitive neuroscience. Finally, behavioral interventions for addiction are discussed. Insights in the
development of control and motivation may help to better understand – and more efficiently
intervene in – vulnerabilities involving control and motivation.
1.2 Dobbs, D. (2011). Beautiful Brains. Moody, Impulsive, Maddening. Why
do teenagers act the way they do? Viewed through the eyes of evolution,
their most exasperating traits may be the key to success as adults.
National Geographic, October 2011.
Teenagers act the way they do, because the brain doesn’t actually grow very much during this
period. The process of maturation continues throughout adolescence. When this development
proceeds normally, we get better at balancing impulse, desire, goals, self-interest, rules, ethics, and
even altruism, generating behavior that is more complex and, sometimes at least, more sensible. But
at times, and especially at first, the brain does this work clumsily. It's hard to get all those new cogs
to mesh.
,In an experiment, neuroimaging was used to study the teen brain, were a simple test was used. She
scanned the brain while they performed a task. Ten-year-olds stink at it, while teens do much better.
But Luna thought the brain scans were much more interesting. Compared with adults, teens tended
to make less use of brain regions that monitor performance, spot errors, plan, and stay focused—
areas the adults seemed to bring online automatically. This let the adults use a variety of brain
resources and better resist temptation, while the teens used those areas less often and more readily
gave in to the impulse to look at the flickering light—just as they're more likely to look away from the
road to read a text message.
However, thee troublesome traits don't really characterize adolescence; they're just what we notice
most because they annoy us or put our children in danger. We have to look at broader traits that
underlie those acts. This upside probably explains why an openness to the new, though it can
sometimes kill the cat, remains a highlight of adolescent development. Even 14- to 17-year-olds—the
biggest risk takers—use the same basic cognitive strategies that adults do, and they usually reason
their way through problems just as well as adults. Contrary to popular belief, they also fully recognize
they're mortal. And, like adults, says Steinberg, "teens actually overestimate risk." So why do teens
take more chances? Teens take more risks not because they don't understand the dangers but
because they weigh risk versus reward differently: In situations where risk can get them something
they want, they value the reward more heavily than adults do.
As Steinberg's driving game suggests, teens respond strongly to social rewards. Physiology and
evolutionary theory alike offer explanations for this tendency. Physiologically, adolescence brings a
peak in the brain's sensitivity to dopamine, a neurotransmitter that appears to prime and fire reward
circuits and aids in learning patterns and making decisions. This helps explain the teen's quickness of
learning and extraordinary receptivity to reward—and his keen, sometimes melodramatic reaction to
success as well as defeat. During adolescence, teens prefer the company of those of their own age
more than ever before.
This adaptive-adolescence view, however accurate, can be tricky to come to terms with—the more
so for parents dealing with teens in their most trying, contrary, or flat-out scary moments.
, 1.3 Sussman, S. (2017). Chapter 1: A general introduction to the concept
of addiction and addictive effects. In Substance and Behavioral Addictions:
Concepts, Causes, and Cures (pp. 3-31). Cambridge University Press.
Substance addiction pertains repetitive intake of a drug or of food, whereas behavioral addiction
pertains engaging in types of behaviors repetitively which are not directly taken into the body such as
gambling or sex. Physiological withdrawal symptoms (the appearance of both physical and
psychological symptoms which are caused by physiological adaptations in the central nervous system
and the brain due to chronic exposure to a substance) were until recently only seen as an ‘addiction’.
Behavioral addictions alter endogenous ligand functions. In Table 1.1 there is a list of 12-step
organizations for substance and behavioral addictions.
The benefits of buying summaries with Stuvia:
Guaranteed quality through customer reviews
Stuvia customers have reviewed more than 700,000 summaries. This how you know that you are buying the best documents.
Quick and easy check-out
You can quickly pay through credit card or Stuvia-credit for the summaries. There is no membership needed.
Focus on what matters
Your fellow students write the study notes themselves, which is why the documents are always reliable and up-to-date. This ensures you quickly get to the core!
Frequently asked questions
What do I get when I buy this document?
You get a PDF, available immediately after your purchase. The purchased document is accessible anytime, anywhere and indefinitely through your profile.
Satisfaction guarantee: how does it work?
Our satisfaction guarantee ensures that you always find a study document that suits you well. You fill out a form, and our customer service team takes care of the rest.
Who am I buying these notes from?
Stuvia is a marketplace, so you are not buying this document from us, but from seller kirstenolthuis. Stuvia facilitates payment to the seller.
Will I be stuck with a subscription?
No, you only buy these notes for $6.43. You're not tied to anything after your purchase.