100% satisfaction guarantee Immediately available after payment Both online and in PDF No strings attached
logo-home
Conversation Analysis summary $8.50   Add to cart

Summary

Conversation Analysis summary

 34 views  2 purchases
  • Course
  • Institution
  • Book

This is a summary of the "Conversation Analysis- an introduction" book. It starts from chapter 3 and goes until chapter 9, these chapters being the only ones used during the course. The summary provides a brief and clear explanation of the concepts and uses examples to make them easier to be unders...

[Show more]

Preview 4 out of 54  pages

  • No
  • Chapter 3 to chapter 9 inclusively
  • June 15, 2022
  • 54
  • 2021/2022
  • Summary
avatar-seller
Chapter 3 - TURN TAKING


- Turns = the basic units of conversation

- “For conversation, preservation of ‘one party talking at a time’ is organizationally
primary” means that turn-taking is organized so as to minimize gaps in which nobody is
talking and overlaps in which more than one person is talking at a time
- Exceptions from the one party talking at a time rule:
➔ Greetings (more people can greet at the same time)
➔ Assessments (often produced in overlap with other talk)
➔ Laughter
- The one party at a time rule can not be achieved by simply waiting for the other person
to finish because:
➔ It would result in the production of a gap
➔ Some “turn-completion signal” (like saying over) would be required and such
signals do not exist in English conversations

- The turn-taking system for conversation is locally managed and partly administered
➔ Locally managed = organized only current and next turn (and not what will
happen in thirty seconds)
➔ Partly administered = there is no “referee” to determine who should speak next
and for how long - the participants work this out themselves
* there are exceptions such as formal debates (where participants are required to talk in a
certain order and for a certain amount of time)

- Turns can not be constructed out of anything ( “has” or “has the park” will not be
considered a turn, but “has the park changed”, will)
- Context is needed to decide whether sth is a turn or not

Eg: 00: Did you say “has” or “jazz”
01: Has

In this case, “has” can be considered to be a turn

- Besides context, intonation, volume and pacing of the talk matters to decide the
existence of a turn

- There is a grammatical well-formedness principle at work here :a turn can be
constructed out of a sharply delimited set of possible unit-types: single words, phrases,
clauses and sentences

, Eg: 35D: you were at the Halloween thing. -> sentential
36S: huh? -> lexical
37D: the Halloween party -> phrasal
38S: right. -> lexical


- Turns are constructed out of units = turn constructional units (TCU)
- A single turn at talk might be built out of several TCUs
- The organization of turns-at-talk is serial (turns follow one another in a series)
- TCUs can be sentential, clausal, phrasal or lexical

Eg:




In lines 07-08 Virginia rejects her mother’s suggestion in three parts, each one of which
is its own TCU:
1. Looks back to mom’s turn locating something problematic in it (Allowance?)
2. Gives ground for rejecting what mom said - the amount is too little (I only get five
dollars a week)
3. Assesses the suggestion (That’s ridiculous)


- Potential next speakers do not wait for the completion of a turn-at-talk. They project its
possible completion and coordinate their own contribution with what the projection allows
them to anticipate -> Points of possible completion create transition-relevant places
(TRP)
- At the possible completion of a unit, transition to the next speaker is relevant -> transition
relevance place
- Both speakers and potential next speakers can orient to the relevance of speaker
transition at a possible completion

, Eg:




First point of possible completion

Second point of possible completion




- Hearers monitor the syntactic, prosodic nd pragmatic features of the current turn to find
that is about to begin, now beginning, continuing, now coming to completion


How turns are distributed in conversation

- Rules: - will be referred to throughout the summary
1. A next speaker may have been selected to speak next by the current turn (eg: an
addressed question) -> the one selected should speak at the first point of
possible completion
2. If no one has been selected by the current turn, at its point of completion any
other part may self-select
3. If no speaker has been selected and no other party self-selects at the possible
completion of the current turn, the current speaker may continue
- These rules are ordered -> crucial to the way in which speakers organize distributions of
turns at talk
Eg: a party wishing to invoke rule 2, must start early, before rule 3 is invoked
When there are more parties (so more could invoke rule 2) ,early starts apply

- Early starts may begin:
➔ At the final sounds of the last word which may constitute a possible complete
utterance
➔ At a recognition point where although the utterance has not been yet completed,
what has been said within and through it has been made available

, - Eg of Overlap vulnerable locations in conversation:
➔ Post-positioned address terms and tags
➔ Lengthened final words

Eg:




Virginia’s “I know” overlaps with “darin’” (addressed term) - she thought the
completion would be at the word “fourteen”
Mom’s “They’re what?” overlaps with Virginia’s “sick” because Virginia extended
her turn a little bit further than it might have otherwise been anticipated
It can also be noticed that in line 03 Mom does not take a break, therefore she
forecloses the possibility of self-selection


Practices of next speaker selection

- Sequence-initiating actions, such as questions, complaints or requests set constraints on
what should be done in the next turn
- First pair parts do not have to be addressed to someone in particular -> do not select a
certain speaker (eg: Who can tell me who wrote this poem?)
- First pair parts do not by themselves allocate next turn to some specific next speaker, so
to do that, they must be combined with some form of address

- How can a speaker show that they are addressing their words to a specific co-
participant?
➔ Address term
➔ Signaled by gaze -> considered explicit means

The benefits of buying summaries with Stuvia:

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Stuvia customers have reviewed more than 700,000 summaries. This how you know that you are buying the best documents.

Quick and easy check-out

Quick and easy check-out

You can quickly pay through credit card or Stuvia-credit for the summaries. There is no membership needed.

Focus on what matters

Focus on what matters

Your fellow students write the study notes themselves, which is why the documents are always reliable and up-to-date. This ensures you quickly get to the core!

Frequently asked questions

What do I get when I buy this document?

You get a PDF, available immediately after your purchase. The purchased document is accessible anytime, anywhere and indefinitely through your profile.

Satisfaction guarantee: how does it work?

Our satisfaction guarantee ensures that you always find a study document that suits you well. You fill out a form, and our customer service team takes care of the rest.

Who am I buying these notes from?

Stuvia is a marketplace, so you are not buying this document from us, but from seller lauramoraru. Stuvia facilitates payment to the seller.

Will I be stuck with a subscription?

No, you only buy these notes for $8.50. You're not tied to anything after your purchase.

Can Stuvia be trusted?

4.6 stars on Google & Trustpilot (+1000 reviews)

75323 documents were sold in the last 30 days

Founded in 2010, the go-to place to buy study notes for 14 years now

Start selling
$8.50  2x  sold
  • (0)
  Add to cart