100% satisfaction guarantee Immediately available after payment Both online and in PDF No strings attached
logo-home
Summary Talent Development & Creativity (PSMOB-7) $7.27   Add to cart

Summary

Summary Talent Development & Creativity (PSMOB-7)

 16 views  4 purchases
  • Course
  • Institution

Summary of 69 pages for the course Talent Development & Creativity (PSMOB-7) at RuG

Preview 5 out of 69  pages

  • October 28, 2023
  • 69
  • 2023/2024
  • Summary
avatar-seller
Summary Talent Development & Creativity

Content
- Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the
acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363-406
- Macnamara, B. N., Hambrick, D. Z., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). Deliberate practice and
performance in music, games, sports, education, and professions: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Science, 25(8), 1608-1618
- Simonton, D. K. (2014). Creative performance, expertise acquisition, individual differences,
and developmental antecedents: An integrative research agenda. Intelligence, 45, 66-73
- Den Hartigh, R. J. R, Van Dijk, M. W. G., Steenbeek, H. W., & Van Geert, P. L. C. (2016). A
Dynamic Network Model to Explain the Development of Excellent Human Performance.
Frontiers in Psychology, 7:532
- Ryan, R. M. & Moller, A. C. (2017). Competence as central, but not sufficient, for high-quality
motivation. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and
motivation: Theory and application (pp. 214-231). Guilford Press
- Van Yperen, N.W. (2021). Achievement goals and self-regulation in the sport context. In:
P.A.M. Van Lange, E.T. Higgins, & A.W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of
Basic Principles, third edition (pp.589-606). Guilford Press
- Van Yperen, N. W. (2022). In the context of a sports match, the goal to win is most
important, right? Suggestive evidence for a hierarchical achievement goal system.
Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 60, 102134
- Zimmerman, B. J., Schunk, D. H., & DiBenedetto, M. K. (2017). The role of self-efficacy and
related beliefs in self-regulation of learning and performance. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D.
S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation: Theory and application (pp. 313–
333). Guilford Press
- Baer, J. & McKool, S. S. (2009). Assessing creativity using the consensual assessment
technique. In: Schreiner, C. S. (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Assessment Technologies,
Methods, and Applications in Higher Education. Hershey, U.S.: Information Science Reference
- Dawes, R. M. (1979). The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making.
American Psychologist, 34, 571-582
- Meijer, R. R., Neumann, M., Hemker, B. T., & Niessen, S. (2020). A tutorial on mechanical
decision-making for personnel and educational selection. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1-8
- Wernimont, P.F. & Campbell, J.P. (1968). Signs, samples and criteria. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 52, 372-376
- Kupers, E., Lehmann-Wermser, A., McPherson, G., & Van Geert, P. (2019). Children’s
creativity: A theoretical framework and systematic review. Review of Educational Research,
89(1), 93-124
- Fleuchaus, E., Kloos, H., Kiefer, A. W., & Silva, P. L. (2020). Complexity in science learning:
Measuring the underlying dynamics of persistent mistakes. The Journal of Experimental
Education, 88(3), 448-469

,- Walton, A. E., Washburn, A., Langland‐Hassan, P., Chemero, A., Kloos, H., & Richardson, M. J.
(2018). Creating time: Social collaboration in music improvisation. Topics in cognitive science,
10(1), 95-119
- Malinin, L. H. (2019). How radical is embodied creativity? Implications of 4E approaches for
creativity research and teaching. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 2372
- Schino, G., van Klaveren, L. M., González, H. G. G., Cox, R. F., Nori, F., & van Heusden, B.
(2022). Measuring Experiences of Art in the Museum: Exploring Methodology for Getting It
Right. Itinera, (23)
- Pelowski M., Markey, P.S., Lauring, J.O., & Leder, H. (2016). Visualizing the impact of art: An
update and comparison of current psychological models of art experience. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 10: 160
- Brinck, I. (2018). Empathy, engagement, entrainment: The interaction dynamics of aesthetic
experience. Cognitive Processes, 19, 201–213
- Cox, R. F. A., Tschacher, W., & van Geert, P. (2023). Modelling Art Experiences Based on the
Theory of Complex Dynamical Systems. OSF Preprint
- Rees, T., Hardy, L., Güllich, A., Abernethy, B., Côté, J., Woodman, T., Montgomery, H., Laing,
S., & Warr, C. (2016). The Great British medalists project: A review of current knowledge on
the development of the world’s best sporting talent. Sports medicine, 46, 1041–1058
- Elferink-Gemser, M.T., Te Wierike, S., Visscher, C. (2018). The acquisition and maintenance of
expertise. Multidisciplinary, longitudinal studies: A perspective from the field of sports. In:
Cambridge Handbook of Expertise (2nd edition). (K.A. Ericsson, R. Hoffman, A. Kozbelt, M.
Williams, eds.), pp. 271-290
- Johnston, K., Wattie, N., Schorer, J., & Baker, J. (2018). Talent identification in sport: a
systematic review. Sports Medicine, 48, 97-109
- Coutinho, P., Mesquita, I., & Fonseca, A. M. (2016). Talent development in sport: A critical
review of pathways to expert performance. International Journal of Sports Science &
Coaching, 11, 279-293
- Pinder, R. A., Renshaw, I., & Davids, K. (2013). The role of representative design in talent
development: a comment on “Talent identification and promotion programmes of Olympic
athletes”. Journal of Sports Sciences, 31, 803-806
- Den Hartigh, R. J. R., Niessen, A. S. M., Frencken, W. G., & Meijer, R. R. (2018). Selection
procedures in sports: Improving predictions of athletes’ future performance. European
Journal of Sport Science, 18, 1191-1198

, The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance
(Ericsson, Krampe & Tesch-Römer, 1993)

Abstract
The framework presented by Ericsson et al. explains expert performance as the end result of
individuals' prolonged efforts to improve performance while negotiating motivational and external
constraints. Most individuals begin in their childhood a regimen of effortful activities (deliberate
practice) designed to optimize improvement. Many characteristics once believed to reflect innate
talent are actually the result of intense practice extended for a minimum of 10 years.

Historical background
Galton --> eminence (exceptional performance) must be transmitted from parents to their offspring.
Eminence is a consequence of inherited ‘natural ability’, which is the conjunction of three elements:
1. Innate capacity.
2. Zeal.
3. Power to do very laborious work.

Natural abilities and other stable characteristics
If genetic factors rigidly determine maximal performance, it is reasonable to assume that these
genetic factors cannot be influenced by practice and training and hence remain stable across time.
But, unique environmental conditions and parental support, rather than talent, may be the
important factors determining the initial onset of training and ultimate performance.

Does practice and experience inevitably lead to maximal performance?
To reach the status of an expert it is sufficient to master existing knowledge and techniques. To make
an eminent achievement one must first achieve the level of an expert and then surpass achievements
of already recognized eminent people and make innovative contributions to the domain. In sum, the
belief that a sufficient amount of experience or practice leads to maximal performance is incorrect.

Preparation time required for attainment of exceptional performance
Expert performance is acquired slowly over a long time as a result of practice and the highest levels
of performance appear to require at least around 10 years of intense prior preparation.
- But, the relation between acquired performance and the amount of practice and experience
was found to be weak to moderate because the current definition of practice is vague.

Characteristics of deliberate practice
Conditions for optimal learning and improvement of performance:
- The motivation to attend to the task and exert effort to improve performance.
- The design of the task should take into account the preexisting knowledge so that the task
can be correctly understood after a brief period of instruction.
- The subjects should receive immediate informative feedback and knowledge of results of
their performance.
- The subjects should repeatedly perform the same or similar tasks.

When these conditions are met, practice improves accuracy and speed of performance.

Comparison of deliberate practice to other types of domain-related activities
Three general types of activities:
- Work --> public performance, competitions, and other activities directly motivated by
external rewards.

, o Constrained in time and require that individuals give their best performance at a
given time.
o External rewards include social recognition, money and prizes, which enables
performers to sustain a living.
- Play --> activities that have no explicit goal and that are inherently enjoyable.
- Deliberate practice --> activities that have been specially designed to improve the current
level of performance.
o It requires effort and is not inherently enjoyable.

Theoretical framework for the acquisition of expert performance
The framework explains how differential levels of performance are attained as a function of
deliberate practice.

Basic assumption (the monotonic benefits assumption) → the amount of time an individual is
engaged in deliberate practice activities is monotonically related to that individual's acquired
performance.

Maximization of deliberate practice extends over a period of at least 10 years and involves
optimization within several constraints:
1. The resource constraint --> deliberate practice requires available time and energy for the
individual as well as access to teachers, training material, and training facilities.
2. The motivational constraint --> engagement in deliberate practice is not inherently
motivating. Performers consider it instrumental in achieving further improvements in
performance.
3. The effort constraint --> deliberate practice is an effortful activity that can be sustained only
for a limited time each day during extended periods without leading to exhaustion.
o Disregard of the effort constraint leads to injury and even failure.

Attaining expert performance
Three phases of development toward adult expertise (Bloom):
1. Begins with an individual's introduction to activities in the domain and ends with the start of
deliberate practice.
2. Begins with an extended period of preparation and ends with the individual's full-time
commitment.
3. Begins with full-time commitment to improving performance and ends when the individual
either can make a living as a professional performer or terminates full-time engagement.
4. Ericsson et al. added a fourth phase to accommodate eminent performance --> individuals go
beyond the knowledge of their teachers to make an innovative contribution to their domain.

Implications for empirical studies
The theoretical framework made three types of predictions:
1. Predictions about the developmental history.
o First, past amount of deliberate practice is directly related to current performance.
Expert performance is not reached with less than 10 years of deliberate practice.
o Second, deliberate practice starts at low levels and increases slowly over time.
2. Predictions about current levels and habits of practice.
o First, the highest improvement of performance is associated with the largest weekly
amounts of deliberate practice. Elite performers practice at a constant level from day
to day to maximize improvement over extended periods of time.
o Second, the daily periods of deliberate practice should be of limited duration with
rest periods in between.

, 3. Predictions about experts' evaluations regarding the nature and role of deliberate practice.
o Deliberate practice will be rated very high on relevance for performance, high on
effort, and comparatively low on inherent enjoyment.

Two studies will be mentioned below:
- Study 1 compared the current and past levels of practice in three groups → elite violinists
with careers and two groups of less accomplished expert violinists.
- Study 2 replicated the results of the first study by comparing expert and amateur pianists.

Summary of results of study 1
The violinists in all groups rated practice alone as the most relevant activity for improving violin
performance. The good violinists had accumulated fewer hours of practice alone by age 18 compared
to the best young violists. Violinists rated sleep as highly relevant for improvement of performance.

Summary of results of study 2
Large differences in the histories of deliberate practice for expert pianists and amateurs. The expert
pianists started 4 years earlier than amateurs. Their average amount of practice increased each year
until it attained its current level, whereas the amateurs maintained their early levels until adulthood.

The framework applied to several domains of expertise
Central thesis of the framework --> expert performance is the result of an extended process of skill
acquisition mediated by large, but not excessive daily amounts of deliberate practice.

Demonstrations of retaining or regaining skill without recent practice are relevant to the framework
of Ericsson et al. for two reasons:
1. Such demonstrations might appear to show a complete dissociation of current practice and
performance. Given that acquired skill resulting from prior accumulated practice cannot be
observed, it could be incorrectly attributed to native talent.
2. The relatively small amount of practice required to maintain or regain a previously acquired
level of performance is different from the massive amount of practice required originally to
attain that level or improve even more. Ericsson et al. assume that the amount of deliberate
practice necessary to specifically maintain earlier attained levels of performance is negligible
for active young experts.

Relation between starting age and performance
The higher the level of attained elite performance, the earlier the age of first exposure as well as the
age of starting deliberate practice.




— = performance associated with an early starting age and a high level of practice.
- - = performance for an equally high level of practice but with a later starting age.
.… = performance associated with the same late starting age and a lower level of practice.

The benefits of buying summaries with Stuvia:

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Stuvia customers have reviewed more than 700,000 summaries. This how you know that you are buying the best documents.

Quick and easy check-out

Quick and easy check-out

You can quickly pay through credit card or Stuvia-credit for the summaries. There is no membership needed.

Focus on what matters

Focus on what matters

Your fellow students write the study notes themselves, which is why the documents are always reliable and up-to-date. This ensures you quickly get to the core!

Frequently asked questions

What do I get when I buy this document?

You get a PDF, available immediately after your purchase. The purchased document is accessible anytime, anywhere and indefinitely through your profile.

Satisfaction guarantee: how does it work?

Our satisfaction guarantee ensures that you always find a study document that suits you well. You fill out a form, and our customer service team takes care of the rest.

Who am I buying these notes from?

Stuvia is a marketplace, so you are not buying this document from us, but from seller svh01. Stuvia facilitates payment to the seller.

Will I be stuck with a subscription?

No, you only buy these notes for $7.27. You're not tied to anything after your purchase.

Can Stuvia be trusted?

4.6 stars on Google & Trustpilot (+1000 reviews)

84251 documents were sold in the last 30 days

Founded in 2010, the go-to place to buy study notes for 14 years now

Start selling
$7.27  4x  sold
  • (0)
  Add to cart