100% satisfaction guarantee Immediately available after payment Both online and in PDF No strings attached
logo-home
Scientific Writing $7.99   Add to cart

Other

Scientific Writing

 1 view  0 purchase

Scientific Writing

Preview 2 out of 6  pages

  • September 20, 2024
  • 6
  • 2024/2025
  • Other
  • Unknown
All documents for this subject (1)
avatar-seller
charlieatlas
Introduction
Urban areas are expanding rapidly worldwide because
the human population is growing very fast, and many
people are moving to such areas (UNDESA 2014). The
effects of urbanisation have huge consequences for the 1194 Journal of Ornithology (2021)
162:1193–11981 3
life history, demography, communities and biodiversity of
birds through the extinction of some species and changes
in the distributions of others (Gil and Brumm 2013; Sepp
et al. 2018). Birds can adapt to urban areas by modify-
ing their behavioural responses (Sol et al. 2013; Audet
et al. 2016; Marzluff 2017), for example, to predators
or competitors (Beckerman et al. 2007; Hasegawa et al.
2010), and also their foraging tactics (Seress et al. 2011;
Møller et al. 2015). Urban populations of some bird spe-
cies have been found to exhibit less neophobia than rural
populations of the same species, e.g. House Sparrow Pas-
ser domesticus and Common Myna Acridotheres tristis
(Liker and Bókony 2009; Sol et al. 2011). Comprehen-
sive studies in Poland have also shown a lesser degree of
neophobia in assemblages of overwintering birds, such as
Great Tit Parus major, in urban than in rural areas. The
most probable explanation for this is that the urbanisa-
tion of birds involves the differential recruitment to urban
habitats of individuals with lower levels of neophobia and
earlier experiences with different food sources (Tryjanow-
ski et al. 2016). Urban environments are rich in various
sources of food for birds, such as refuse dumps, leftover
pet food, waste human food and bird feeders (Tryjanowski
et al. 2015; Ciach and Fröhlich 2017).
Neophobic behaviour may be influenced by the fea-
tures of an object such as its colour (Gamberale-Stille et al.
2007), and many birds possess a preference for, or aversion
to, food of different colours (Mastrota and Mench 1995).
Many diurnal birds have the most complex (tetrachromatic)
colour vision of all vertebrates and use it in every aspect of
life, e.g. for recognising and discriminating objects (Osorio
and Vorobyev 2008). Preferences for particular food col-
ours have been reported in birds (Hartley et al. 2000; Duan
et al. 2014), but studies relating to the colours of bird feeders
have focussed mainly on the preferences of hummingbirds
Trochilidae. Experimental studies at such feeders have not
shown that hummingbirds consistently prefer one particular
colour over another, although earlier studies did suggest a
preference for red, because this colour was associated with
the red flowers of nectar-rich plant species (Cronk and Ojeda
2008; Handelman and Kohn 2014). It is likely that other fac-


1

, tors, such as location, previous experience and nectar qual-
ity, are more important in governing the choices of these
birds. Apart from these studies of hummingbirds, only Roth-
ery et al. (2017) demonstrated that garden birds in the UK
preferred feeders of particular colours: higher numbers of
several species visited silver and green feeders than red and
yellow ones. Those authors stated that the former colours
could have been preferred because feeders of these colours
are often used in the UK, so the birds are familiar with them.
There may, however, be a wider but unknown reason behind
this preference for the colour green: Brown (2006) stated
that tits preferred green nesting boxes to brown ones, yet
brown is the colour of their natural nesting sites. The colour
preferences of birds are, therefore, not well understood.
The aim of this paper was to examine the colour prefer-
ence of two populations of birds: one that was exposed to
novel items (urban), and the other not exposed to such items
(rural—naïve), the hypothesis being that previous exposure
affects the level of neophobia. We used bird feeders of two
colours—yellow and green—in this investigation because
birds are sensitive to these colours, and because of the
results obtained by Rothery et al. (2017). We hypothesised
that urban birds come into contact with novel, coloured
objects such as bird feeders, garbage and other anthropo-
genic items more often than rural birds. Hence, we predicted
that urban birds, potentially less neophobic, would feed more
often at yellow feeders than their rural counterparts. For the
same reason, birds in an urban habitat would not display
a preference for any particular colour when approaching a
feeder for the first time. We conducted the experiments in
towns and villages in east-central Poland.
Methods
Data collection
Data were collected during December–February in the
winters of 2018/2019 and 2019/2020 in 5 towns (urban)
and 12 villages (rural) in east-central Poland. The popula-
tions of the towns (Siedlce, Węgrów, Sokołów Podlaski,
Łuków, and Siemiatycze) ranged from 12 600 to 78 000
(mean = 31 000), whereas the villages had from 136 to 550
(mean = 232) inhabitants (https:// bip. gov. pl). Across the
years, a total of 43 experimental trials, each in a different
place (21 and 22 in rural and urban areas, respectively), were
carried out in good weather, i.e. no snow or rain, or strong
wind. The experimental sites were set up in places where
small passerines, especially tits, were frequently observed,
but no closer than 50 m from existing feeders. These sites
were chosen during a slow drive around the study area. The
minimum distance between two adjacent experimental sites
was 1 km. Two feeders of the same type were used in the
experiment: they were shaped like a small house with a roof
and were mounted on a 120 cm-tall tripod. The two dif-


2

The benefits of buying summaries with Stuvia:

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Stuvia customers have reviewed more than 700,000 summaries. This how you know that you are buying the best documents.

Quick and easy check-out

Quick and easy check-out

You can quickly pay through credit card or Stuvia-credit for the summaries. There is no membership needed.

Focus on what matters

Focus on what matters

Your fellow students write the study notes themselves, which is why the documents are always reliable and up-to-date. This ensures you quickly get to the core!

Frequently asked questions

What do I get when I buy this document?

You get a PDF, available immediately after your purchase. The purchased document is accessible anytime, anywhere and indefinitely through your profile.

Satisfaction guarantee: how does it work?

Our satisfaction guarantee ensures that you always find a study document that suits you well. You fill out a form, and our customer service team takes care of the rest.

Who am I buying these notes from?

Stuvia is a marketplace, so you are not buying this document from us, but from seller charlieatlas. Stuvia facilitates payment to the seller.

Will I be stuck with a subscription?

No, you only buy these notes for $7.99. You're not tied to anything after your purchase.

Can Stuvia be trusted?

4.6 stars on Google & Trustpilot (+1000 reviews)

85443 documents were sold in the last 30 days

Founded in 2010, the go-to place to buy study notes for 14 years now

Start selling
$7.99
  • (0)
  Add to cart