100% satisfaction guarantee Immediately available after payment Both online and in PDF No strings attached
logo-home
School psychology - article summary $6.42
Add to cart

Summary

School psychology - article summary

1 review
 122 views  10 purchases
  • Course
  • Institution

School psychology summary articles

Preview 4 out of 40  pages

  • October 25, 2020
  • 40
  • 2019/2020
  • Summary

1  review

review-writer-avatar

By: melvarluv21 • 3 year ago

avatar-seller
SUMMARY OF THE ARTICLES FOR THE SPECIALIZATION
COURSE AT LEIDEN UNIVERSITY




A Developmental Perspective on Executive Function – Best & Miller – Lecture
1
Training the developing brain: A neurocognitive perspective – Jolles & Crone –
lecture 2
Creativity Development in Adolescence: Insight from Behavior, Brain, and
Training Studies - Kleinbeukker et al. – Lecture 3
Computers in mathematics education – Training the mental number line –
Weel 4 – Mueller

Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis – Roth et al. – Lecture 5
Dynamic Assessment – Elliott et al. – Lecture 5
Massive IQ Gains in 14 Nations: What IQ Tests really measure – Flynn –
Lecture 5
Intrinsic and extrinsic school motivation as a function of age: the mediating
role of autonomy support – Gillet et. Al – week 8


Need supportive teaching in practice: a narrative analysis in schools with
contrasting educational approaches – Stroet et. Al – week 8
A 2 x 2 Achievement Goal Framework - Elliot et. Al – week 8




1

,A Developmental Perspective on Executive Function – Best & Miller – Lecture
1
This review paper examines theoretical and methodological issues in the construction of a
developmental perspective on executive function (EF) in childhood and adolescence. Unlike most
reviews of EF, which focus on preschoolers, this review focuses on studies that include large age
ranges. It outlines the development of the foundational components of EF—inhibition, working
memory, and shifting. Cognitive and neurophysiological assessments show that although EF emerges
during the first few years of life, it continues to strengthen significantly throughout childhood and
adolescence. The components vary somewhat in their developmental trajectories. The paper relates
the findings to longstanding issues of development (e.g., developmental sequences, trajectories, and
processes) and suggests research needed for constructing a developmental framework encompassing
early childhood through adolescence.
Executive functions: cognitive processes that underlie goal-directed behavior and are orchestrated
by activity within the prefrontal cortex (PFC)
 Research of EF has three limitations that pose difficulties for constructing a truly
developmental account of EF
1) Most research on the development of EF has examined narrow age ranges, for example,
ages 2 to 5
2) Most has focused on preschoolers, perhaps because rapid improvements occur during
the preschool and early school years on EF tasks
3) We have little info about the processes by which children move from one level to
another, especially processes operating after age 5
Consequently, despite the large literature on EF in children, there is no truly developmental account
of EF across childhood and adolescence.
 Purpose of this paper: To begin to construct such an account, which distinguishes it from
previously published reviews of EF
Theoretical and methodological challenges
A main challenge is the lack of agreement concerning whether EF is a unitary construct or a set of
independent components. One prominent theoretical framework integrates these opposing
perspectives by suggesting that the EF construct consists of interrelated, but distinct, components—
described as the “unity and diversity of EF”.
 Some research with children has investigated the EF construct and has found at least partial
support for an integrative framework. Hughes sought to expand a previous finding that EF
consists of dissociable components in older children. She extracted three distinct factors—
attentional flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory—from preschoolers’
performance on several EF tasks, suggesting that EF components are differentiated even at a
young age
 When sample does include older school-age children and adolescents, methodological
challenges can arise:
1) To avoid ceiling effects researchers often use complex EF tasks that likely tap into
multiple executive functions, a problem of task impurity
2) Related to first, tasks used across an age range often are not uniform. Tasks too difficult
for the younger participants sometimes are only administered to the older ones, which
makes comparisons across age groups difficult. Or very different tasks are used to assess
a particular dimension for preschoolers and older children
2

,  Keeping this issue in mind, we utilize Miyake’s “unity and diversity” theoretical framework to
focus on the “foundational” EFs—inhibition, information updating and monitoring (WM), and
shifting—in part because several frequently used cognitive tasks ostensibly tap into each
dimension. In using this framework to address EF development from early childhood through
adolescence, we keep in mind current developmental theories of EF. In general, most of
these theories depict EF development as involving an increasing ability to resolve conflict.
 Focus on those studies that most clearly address developmental issues (those that examine
both preschoolers and school-age children, or school-age children and adolescents, address
the order of acquisition of different aspects of EF, or examine possible developmental
processes. This approach permits us to detect developmental trajectories, sequences, and
processes.
 Has been known for year that patients with PFC damage can have EF deficits, yet normal IQ
 More recent: That the PFC coordinated posterior cortical and subcortical brain activity
via excitatory and inhibitory pathways. Moreover, PFC activity holds relevant information
in WM and prevents distracting information from entering WM
Also know from structural imaging studies, that PFC development, like brain development more
generally, consists of both progressive (e.g., myelination, neuron proliferation, synaptogenesis,) and
regressive changes (e.g., cell death, synaptic pruning). The PFC matures later in adolescence as
evidenced by further loss of gray matter, unlike many other brain regions that mature earlier (e.g.,
regions involved in attention, motor and sensory processing, and speech and language development).
During this time, progressive and regressive changes (largely myelination and synaptic pruning,
respectively) occur concomitantly and are driven in part by the child’s experiences—the result being
“efficient networks of neuronal connections”.


Foundational Executive Functions
Inhibition
 Inhibition is considered foundational for EF, however most inhibition tasks are not pure
measures of inhibition
 Garon et al. (2008) distinguished simple from complex response inhibition tasks based on
whether WM also is needed
 Simple response inhibition required a minimal amount of WM (making it one of the
purest forms of inhibition)  shows its rudiments during infancy
 Complex response inhibition also requires substantial WM by requiring that an arbitrary
rule be held in mind and/or by requiring that the child inhibit one response (prepotent or
not) and produce an alternative response (day/night task)
 Also, differentiation between delay tasks which require withholding a proponent response,
from conflict tasks, which require the child to make a response that conflicts with a
prepotent response


Age differences
 Rapid movements in early childhood on a variety of complex response inhibition tasks.
Despite their apparent similarities, different conflict tasks show different ages of mastery,
perhaps indicating different cognitive demands.



3

, Lurias hand game: requires children to make a fist when shown a finger and vice versa, the most
improvement typically occurs between age 3 and 4
Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): complex response inhibition task used frequently with
preschool children. DCCS creates a prepotent response during the pre-switch phase that must later
be inhibited. The child is shown a deck of cards that vary on two dimensions— shape (e.g., rabbit
versus battleship) and color (e.g., red versus blue). During the pre-switch phase, the child must sort
the cards according to one dimension (e.g., color; “If it’s red, it goes here; if it’s blue, it goes here”). In
the post-switch phase, the child is asked to sort the cards by the other dimension (e.g., shape).
Similar to other conflict tasks, reductions in perseveration occur from age 3 through age 4
Advanced DCCS: Third sorting dimension is added, if there is a star on the card, the child should sort
by color, but if there is not a star, the child should sort by shape
 Findings of further improvement in inhibition after age 5 are mixed
 Other studies find further development after age 8. Interestingly, many of these studies have
utilized computerized tasks such as the Go/no-go task or the continuous performance task
(CPT), both of which require a response to certain stimuli and inhibition of response to other
stimuli
Go/No-Go task: child must respond (by pressing a designated keyboard button) only to “go” stimuli
(e.g., all letters except X) and inhibit response to the “no-go” stimulus (e.g., the letter X)
 Studies suggest that the stage of execution is a factor in inhibition difficulty: Terminating an
already executed response appears to be more difficult than inhibiting a response that has
yet to be executed or is in an earlier stage of execution
 The fact that several studies have found improved performance beyond early childhood on
ostensibly simple response inhibition tasks (e.g., anti-saccade task)—or at least on tasks (e.g.,
Go/No-go task) simpler than conflict tasks—challenges the notion that performance on these
tasks matures early on
 In summary, the first leap in attaining inhibition appears in the preschool years. By age 4
children show signs of successful performance on both simple (i.e., pure response inhibition)
and complex inhibition tasks (i.e., response inhibition plus alternative response). By the same
age children can operate on a bi-dimensional card by one dimension and then inhibit that to
use the second dimension (i.e., successfully perform DCCS). Inhibition continues to improve,
particularly from age 5 to 8 and particularly for tasks that combine inhibition and WM, but
also at later ages, especially on computerized tasks
 Unlike the early improvements these are no fundamental changes in cognition 
refinements that seem to involve quantitative improvements in accuracy, perhaps due to
an increasing efficiency to override prepotent responses




Evidence from neuroscience
 One method to examine the neural response underlying response inhibition is to measure
the brain’s electrical activity via EEG
 EEG studies with older children indicate continued localization of brain activity from middle
childhood to adulthood



4

The benefits of buying summaries with Stuvia:

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Guaranteed quality through customer reviews

Stuvia customers have reviewed more than 700,000 summaries. This how you know that you are buying the best documents.

Quick and easy check-out

Quick and easy check-out

You can quickly pay through credit card or Stuvia-credit for the summaries. There is no membership needed.

Focus on what matters

Focus on what matters

Your fellow students write the study notes themselves, which is why the documents are always reliable and up-to-date. This ensures you quickly get to the core!

Frequently asked questions

What do I get when I buy this document?

You get a PDF, available immediately after your purchase. The purchased document is accessible anytime, anywhere and indefinitely through your profile.

Satisfaction guarantee: how does it work?

Our satisfaction guarantee ensures that you always find a study document that suits you well. You fill out a form, and our customer service team takes care of the rest.

Who am I buying these notes from?

Stuvia is a marketplace, so you are not buying this document from us, but from seller nl_summaries. Stuvia facilitates payment to the seller.

Will I be stuck with a subscription?

No, you only buy these notes for $6.42. You're not tied to anything after your purchase.

Can Stuvia be trusted?

4.6 stars on Google & Trustpilot (+1000 reviews)

53068 documents were sold in the last 30 days

Founded in 2010, the go-to place to buy study notes for 14 years now

Start selling
$6.42  10x  sold
  • (1)
Add to cart
Added