100% tevredenheidsgarantie Direct beschikbaar na betaling Zowel online als in PDF Je zit nergens aan vast
logo-home
Uitwerking hoorcolleges aantekeningen Complex Systems Theory €4,49   In winkelwagen

College aantekeningen

Uitwerking hoorcolleges aantekeningen Complex Systems Theory

2 beoordelingen
 152 keer bekeken  4 keer verkocht

Alle hoorcolleges van Complex Systems Theory zijn uitgewerkt (inclusief informatieve afbeeldingen) en extra aantekeningen

Voorbeeld 4 van de 37  pagina's

  • 7 april 2020
  • 37
  • 2019/2020
  • College aantekeningen
  • Onbekend
  • Alle colleges
Alle documenten voor dit vak (13)

2  beoordelingen

review-writer-avatar

Door: aniquenies • 3 jaar geleden

review-writer-avatar

Door: Barbara21 • 4 jaar geleden

avatar-seller
JuliaM1
Lectures Complex Systems Theory
Lecture 1A Introduction to complex systems
Complex Systems: a general definition
There are a number of characteristic features that are shared by almost all complex systems. A
complex system can often be seen as a large collection of small elements that interact with each other
at a micro-level. Such elements may be atoms in physics, molecules or cells in biology, or consumers
in socioeconomics. However, ‘more is different’ (Anderson, 1972) in complex systems. Phenomena
observed at a global, macro-level, typically cannot be reduced to the properties of the constituent
elements: there are emergent properties that arise through ‘self-organizing’ local interactions. This is
in sharp contrast to the classical reductionistic idea that nature can only be understood by reducing or
decomposing its processes into elementary building blocks that can be studied independently. The
concept of ‘Complexity’ has been introduced as the associated paradigm shift in the study of natural
phenomena.

In science we tend to look at only one element. This approach doesn’t work in complex systems.

Differences with linear science
Linear systems vs. complex systems:
Components vs. interactions
Linear change vs. nonlinear change (complex systems evolve over time)
One scale vs. multiple scales
- Micro/macro
- Spatial/temporal
Static vs. dynamic
Single vs. multiple causality
Reductionism vs. self-organization and emergence

Properties of complex systems and examples
Complicated Systems vs. Complex Systems
Complicated systems: Components, dynamical system,
predictable system, it can perfectly be understood.
Complex system: When you have to understand a little
part of the system, you have to understand the working
of the whole system (e.g., nervous system).

Components: When you know the components, you
don’t know that much about behavior. But you do when
you study the interaction of the components.

Reductionism:
- Reduces complicated problems into simpler parts.
- System behavior = sum of component behavior.

Interactions:
Self-organization:
- Local interactions between components
- Global emergent system behavior
- Does not reduce to the behavior of the components

, Everything what we are doing is dependent of all
the components doing other things.
There’s not a single cause for a change in
behavior. There is not one component that
doesn’t work in for example dyslectic readers.

Interactions: behavioral, neuro, and genetic
studies at the group level often mask important
findings at the individual level. Months and
years, even decades, days and weeks and
seconds and minutes.



On average you can see a difference between two groups. But if you
look into a brain between two different people, you can see different
brain activity.

What did we learn so far from linear statistics?
What we do often in our field is categorizing people into groups. And we
end up with statements that are general.
Broad statements about groups
- Women are more emotional than men
- Baby’s don’t have a ToM, whereas toddlers do
- Lesbians only have sex with women
- Adolescents are more impulsive than young adults
- People with ADHD have weakly developed EF
- Children with ASD display repetitive behavior
- Teachers are prejudiced against pupils from low-SES backgrounds
Statements are often linear
- The older one gets → the less impulsive the behavior will be
- The weaker EF → the stronger ADHD
- The more repetitive behavior → the clearer the autism

What did we learn so far from linear statistics?
Compare means: Mean EFadhd < Mean EFcontrol. 20(SD=3.4) < 24(SD=4.2). T test is significant, p<.05
Compute correlation: Age * Impulsiveness: r = -0.30, p < .05, 9% variance explained.
Run ANOVA: IV = Gender (Female vs. Male) and Group (Autistic vs. Control) DV = Score on a test of
Theory of Mind (ToM)
Effect of Gender: ToMfemale > ToMmale 3% of variance explained Effect of Group: ToMautistic < ToMcontrol
10% of variance explained

You can explain just a small part about something; 91% is still not explained!
When you work with one individual, you can do not much with these statistics.

Scope of traditional statistics:
- Only concern the group, not the individual.
- Linear relationships (e.g., correlation, t test, ANOVA, regression)
- Components explain behavior (IV + Noise → DV): Autism + noise → ToM
- Independent components added together explain behavior: 3% of the variance caused by
gender + 10 % by the group = 13% in total.
- Observed score = true score + noise; In formula: X1 = T + ε1
Scope of complexity theory:
- Concern group and individual, measured over time. When you measure a lot of induvial, then
you still can say something about groups.
- Nonlinear relationships (e.g., phase transitions, CUSP catastrophe, small cause large effect
and vice versa)
- Interactions explain behavior (local interactions →global pattern, emergence)
- Dependent components not A → B, entire system may matter
- Observed score = dynamical change process; in formula: Xt+1 = f(Xt)

,Linear: A change in X is associated with a proportional change in Y.




Nonlinear: A change in Y is not proportional to X. Far more common than linearity in human behavior:
finger movement, ‘eep’-to-‘pee’, transitions from walking to running and perceptual ambiguity.




 Static: Typically, each data point represents one measured value
for one person.
Dynamic: Typically, each data point represents a change process
(many values measured over time) of one individual. Yt+1 = f(Yt) →

Example: Vocabulary development. Nonlinear and dynamic, e.g.
vocabulary spurt.



 Individual trajectories
Group level data: aggregated
over individual trajectories →

Causality in complex systems: it
is not A → B. But a dynamic
explanation.

Example: Mood dynamics. Depressive symptoms can fluctuate enormously.




Left is individual data. Aggregated: M
= 4.6 and SD = 2.6. How meaningful
is M?
The mean is the value that occurs the
least →

, Lecture 1B Diversities A
Why care about diversity?
Almost everything you’ve learned so far is based on population statistic. Interventions (clinical,
educational, youth care etc.) are effective on average. But they are not effective for everyone…




Diversity as Error (traditional view)
Mean = Norm(al)
Deviation (i.e., diversity) = error
Average represents most.

Diversity is a kind of error/deviation. We
often use the mean to score people.
Average represent most of the people. When you design something on average, it will help most of the
people. When you are a weirdo, you have bad luck; this intervention won’t work for you.

The average brain is created by individual brain patterns. So not one individual matches with the
average brain

What is happening here?
Dimensionality: Average size includes:
- Length
- Arm length
- Leg length
- Neck circumference
- Thigh circumference
- Wrist circumference
- …
Nobody scores average on multidimensional constructs! Other examples of dimensions: symptoms, IQ
subdomains. Principle of jaggedness: no individual corresponds to average.

Main message 1: The average person does not exist. Nobody scores average when there are many
dimensions involved.

Diversity as rank: the case of correlation.
Individuals are deviations from average with
certain rank. Two variables are positively
correlated when a high rank in X is
associated to a high rank in Y. Misleading
correlations: typing speed.

Main message 2: Group differences do not
generalize to individual processes. Between-
subject and within-subject variation are different. Between-subject results have no reliable implications
for interventions. All research questions concerning change demand within-subject analysis.

Voordelen van het kopen van samenvattingen bij Stuvia op een rij:

Verzekerd van kwaliteit door reviews

Verzekerd van kwaliteit door reviews

Stuvia-klanten hebben meer dan 700.000 samenvattingen beoordeeld. Zo weet je zeker dat je de beste documenten koopt!

Snel en makkelijk kopen

Snel en makkelijk kopen

Je betaalt supersnel en eenmalig met iDeal, creditcard of Stuvia-tegoed voor de samenvatting. Zonder lidmaatschap.

Focus op de essentie

Focus op de essentie

Samenvattingen worden geschreven voor en door anderen. Daarom zijn de samenvattingen altijd betrouwbaar en actueel. Zo kom je snel tot de kern!

Veelgestelde vragen

Wat krijg ik als ik dit document koop?

Je krijgt een PDF, die direct beschikbaar is na je aankoop. Het gekochte document is altijd, overal en oneindig toegankelijk via je profiel.

Tevredenheidsgarantie: hoe werkt dat?

Onze tevredenheidsgarantie zorgt ervoor dat je altijd een studiedocument vindt dat goed bij je past. Je vult een formulier in en onze klantenservice regelt de rest.

Van wie koop ik deze samenvatting?

Stuvia is een marktplaats, je koop dit document dus niet van ons, maar van verkoper JuliaM1. Stuvia faciliteert de betaling aan de verkoper.

Zit ik meteen vast aan een abonnement?

Nee, je koopt alleen deze samenvatting voor €4,49. Je zit daarna nergens aan vast.

Is Stuvia te vertrouwen?

4,6 sterren op Google & Trustpilot (+1000 reviews)

Afgelopen 30 dagen zijn er 73918 samenvattingen verkocht

Opgericht in 2010, al 14 jaar dé plek om samenvattingen te kopen

Start met verkopen
€4,49  4x  verkocht
  • (2)
  Kopen